Brit-Am 
 DNA and 
 Anthropology Updates 
Updates in DNA studies along with Anthropological Notes of general interest with a particular emphasis on points pertinent to the study of Ancient Israelite Ancestral Connections to Western Peoples as explained in Brit-Am studies.
Brit-Am
Research
Revelation
Reconciliation
The Brit-Am Rose
Official Symbol of Brit-Am
BAMAD no. 48
Brit-Am Anthropology and DNA Update
9th March 2009, 13 Adar 5769
Contents:
1. Mice Prove that the Same Color Change
may be Unrelated to a Common Ancestry!
2. Distance from Africa, not climate explains human variation.
3. White "Black" Africans in South Africa
Are albinos smarter?
1. Mice Prove that the Same Color Change
may be Unrelated to a Common Ancestry!
The genetics of skin pigmentation in beach mice from different beaches
http://yannklimentidis.blogspot.com/
The Genetic Basis of Phenotypic Convergence in Beach Mice: Similar Pigment
Patterns but Different Genes
Brit-Am Summarization:
The somewhat complicated language of the
Abstract below tells us that different types of mice in a specific area change
their coloring in accordance to the environment. They change their coloring in
the same way even though they are not related to each other. A certain type of
mice in one area may be of the same type as that somewhere else but his coloring
could be different. His coloring in effect could be the same as that of other
mice in his same region who from other considerations may be of an entirely
different type.
Cynthia C. Steiner, Holger R?pler, Linda M. Boettger, Torsten Sch?eberg and Hopi
E. Hoekstra
Molecular Biology and Evolution 2009 26(1):35-45
Abstract: Convergent evolution is a widespread phenomenon seen in diverse
organisms inhabiting similar selective environments. However, it is unclear if
similar phenotypes are produced by the same or different genes and mutations.
Here we analyze the molecular mechanisms underlying convergent pigment pattern
among subspecies of the beach mouse (Peromyscus polionotus) inhabiting the Gulf
and Atlantic coasts of Florida. In these two geographic regions, separated by
more than 300 km, "beach mice" have lighter colored coats than do their mainland
counterparts, produced by natural selection for camouflage against the pale
coastal sand dunes. We measured color pattern in eight beach mouse subspecies
and showed that three of the Gulf Coast subspecies are more phenotypically
similar to an Atlantic coast subspecies than to their Gulf Coast neighbors.
However, light-colored beach mice do not form a monophyletic group. Previous
results implicated a single derived amino acid change in the melanocortin-1
receptor (Mc1r) as a major contributor to pigment pattern in the Gulf Coast
beach mice; despite phenotypic similarities, the derived Mc1r allele was not
found in the Atlantic coast beach mouse populations. Here we show that Atlantic
coast beach mice have high levels of Mc1r polymorphism but they lack unique
alleles. Functional assays revealed that single amino acid mutations segregating
in Atlantic coast beach mice do not cause any change in Mc1r activity compared
with the activity of Mc1r from dark-colored mice. These joint results show that
convergent pigment patterns in recently diverged beach mouse subspecies, whose
developmental constraints are presumably similar, have evolved through a
diversity of genetic mechanisms.
2. Distance from Africa, not climate
explains human variation.
Brit-Am Summarization:
The variation considered
here refers to physical changes rather than color. We hold this is determined by
environment.
"Environment" includes location i.e. where one is on the planet earth has an
effect of its own.
Distance from Africa, not climate explains human variation.
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19129123
Lia Betti, Francis Balloux, William Amos, Tsunehiko Hanihara, Andrea Manica
Proc Roy Soc B Early online
Abstract: The relative importance of ancient demography and climate in
determining worldwide patterns of human within-population phenotypic diversity
is still open to debate. Several morphometric traits have been argued to be
under selection by climatic factors, but it is unclear whether climate affects
the global decline in morphological diversity with increasing geographical
distance from sub-Saharan Africa. Using a large database of male and female
skull measurements, we apply an explicit framework to quantify the relative role
of climate and distance from Africa. We show that distance from sub-Saharan
Africa is the sole determinant of human within-population phenotypic diversity,
while climate plays no role. By selecting the most informative set of traits, it
was possible to explain over half of the worldwide variation in phenotypic
diversity. These results mirror those previously obtained for genetic markers
and show that "bones and molecules" are in perfect agreement for humans.
3. White "Black" Africans in South
Africa
Are albinos smarter?
http://evoandproud.blogspot.com/search?updated-min=2008-01-01T00%3A00%3A00-05%3A00&updated-max=2009-01-01T00%3A00%3A00-05%3A00&max-results=50
Extracts:
Steve Sailer asks whether albinos are smarter. He points to a 1997 New York
Times article on albinism in southern Africa:
John M. Makumbe is a professor of political science. Richard Nyathi is chief
librarian for a Government ministry. Stanley S. Gunda is a senior financial
officer in another ministry. Not so long ago, they might have been killed at
birth.
Messrs. Makumbe, Nyathi and Gunda are albinos. In Africa, far more than on any
other continent, that is a lifelong curse. They lack the gene that codes the
skin pigment melanin, and they are very nearsighted. As white-skinned men in a
black society, they are shunned and feared as the products of witchcraft,
taunted by children and drunks as ''peeled potatoes,'' ''monkeys'' and
''ghosts.''
There is a stereotype that all albinos are intelligent and accomplished, as
these three men are. But Professor Makumbe said successful albinos were ''a
teeny-weeny minute number. "Most, he added, languish at home without education
because they cannot see the blackboard at school or because their parents, told
such children die young, will not pay for their schooling."
All three men did well in school, despite vision problems. The genetic
differences that cause albinism also change the connections between the optic
nerves and the brain. Many albinos have nystagmus -- ''dancing eyeballs'' -- and
myopia that, even with thick glasses, can only be corrected to about 20-200.
''Albinos seem more intelligent because they try harder,'' Mr. Nyathi said.
''You have to get out of your seat, go up to the board, squint, write two
sentences, go back, and still finish the test in the same time as the others.''
Interestingly, the medical literature used to assume that albinism produces
mental retardation. Not only are albinos not retarded, they actually seem to do
better than average. Manganyl et al. (1974) found they had higher levels of
intellectual maturity than participants in a control group. Fulcher et al.
(1995) similarly observed significantly higher achievement among albinos in
reading, spelling, and arithmetic skills.
Nonetheless, standard IQ tests show no difference between albinos and controls
(Beckham 1946; Estrada-Hernandez & Harper 2007; Fulcher et al. 1995; Kutzbach et
al. 2008). It looks like albinos have the same intellectual potential but make
better use of it.
This seems to be Dr. Kromberg's explanation: ''Albinos have normal I.Q.'s but a
higher capability. I think it's because they don't play in the sun all day. They
stay inside and do their homework.''
On the other hand, IQ tests do not measure all aspects of intellectual
performance. This is notably the case with ?executive function,? i.e., the
ability to resist habit, the speed at which you can change the focus of your
attention or the contents of your working memory, and the speed at which you can
change your goals and respond appropriately. If we look at differences in these
functions between identical twins, about 86% to 100% of the variability seems to
be heritable (Friedman et al., in press).
Looking through the anthropological literature, I came across two references for
higher intellectual attainment among albinos, and one for lower attainment:
Albinos are smart, clean, nice, and pretty. There is nothing wrong with them.
[comments from traditional Hopis] (Woolf & Dukepoo 1969)
Some people think that we are useless because of our poor eyesight and skin
condition. Others think we are idiots [Comments from Dominican Republic albinos]
(Westhoff 1993)
... The mentality of Moon-children [albino Cuna Indians] appears to be normal,
and no less than 14 of them have competed in formal school classes. It is a
common belief among the Cuna that since they are unable to compete physically,
they strive that much harder to succeed intellectually. (Keeler 1953)
For what it's worth, the last reference also mentioned an apparent difference in
head shape:
Head shape of Moon-children appears to be brachycephalic in a higher percentage
than normal. No statistical data on this subject were collected, but in addition
to my own observations the Indians themselves have remarked about this apparent
correlation. (Keeler 1953)
BAMAD Archives
Join the Brit-Am Ephraimite Discussion Group
Just Send an
e-mail
with "Subscribe"
in the Subject Line
Main Page
Offerings and Publications
Return to
Question and Answer
Table of Contents